Free Energy Principle
Neuroscientist Karl Friston on the Markov blanket, Bayesian model evidence, and different global brain theorie...
Educational neuroscience is an attempt to use new findings about the mechanisms of learning in the brain that are emerging from neuroscience and to understand how they may be translated into implications in the classroom to improve educational outcomes of children. To understand why educational neuroscience is relevant or important, or new, I think, we have to step back a bit and think about what we’re trying to do in the field of psychology.
In psychology we’re basically trying to understand how the mind works, and there’s a bunch of reasons why we might want to do it. We might want to predict how people are going to behave, change, how they’re going to behave in the classroom, we want to understand how to teach children, how to give them skills, how to give them knowledge, how to turn out good citizens for the economy.
So how do we understand how the mind works when all we can see is people’s behavior from the outside? We have to figure out what’s going on inside their minds, and all we can see is behavior. What we do is we use a standard scientific approach: we try and infer hidden calls or entities, mechanisms that we can’t see based on the overt behavior that we see in adults and children. We’ve been pretty good at doing that: we’ve come up with things like working memory, and attention, and perception, and emotions and things like that.
The drawback is that it turns out that as scientists we don’t have very good imagination when it comes to thinking about what might be going on inside people’s heads. We tend to think about the technology of the day and think that it might be a good kind of metaphor for what’s going on inside our head.
You can trace this back through history, you can look at the idea that the mind works like a steam engine (Freudian idea) or that the mind works like a telephone exchange, or that memory works like a hologram. I think, the most recent version of this type of mechanistic metaphor of the mind is the desktop computer: we think that maybe the mind works like a desktop computer. And if you think about the phrase, “working memory”, that’s exactly the same phrase that we use to talk about keeping things in mind and about a computer, that it has working memory. I’m upgrading my computer to 10 gigabytes of working memory. The problem of thinking of the mind in terms of the working of a desktop computer is that’s not really paying attention to how the brain works and what the brain finds easy to do and what it finds hard to do.
This is part of a broader picture: as we are getting a deeper understanding of brain function, we’re beginning to see the influence of neuroscience in multiple different areas, such as understanding of economics or understanding of law, understanding security – these kind of things as a range of wider influences of neuroscience. With rise of understanding of how the brain functions and the emergence of the field of neuroscience we’re beginning to see a wider influence of neuroscience on different areas within society, including how neuroscience impacts on economics, how neuroscience impacts on law.
The area I work in is the impact of neuroscience on education: trying to understand how brain mechanisms of learning constrain, how children learn, what they learn, how quickly they can learn and what things we can learn at different ages. I’m trying to do this transfer of neuroscience research to education and to be guided by educators and what the key questions are in neuroscience, and as I work in this field, some things strike me about teachers. So first off, teachers are the experts in the classroom: they’ve got years of experience, they have an intuitive feel for what works with children, but they don’t have a training in the mechanisms that underpin learning in children. That’s an interesting comparison, say, to doctors who would have an extended training on how the body works before they sit down with a patient and diagnose a disease. There’s been a long history of accumulation of knowledge in teachers about what works in the classroom, but that kind of trial and error accumulation of practice sometimes leads to is fashion in education: techniques that are used one year, but a decade later you’re using something different. And it also lets in the possibility of techniques in education that really don’t have any evidential base. So the idea of bringing in a deeper understanding and mechanisms underpinning learning and conveying that to teachers is an attractive one.
The other thing that strikes me about teachers, at least in the UK, is that they’re very excited about neuroscience, they’re enthusiastic about it and implicitly that suggests they understand the importance of mechanisms of learning as one aspect of knowledge that should inform their practice. Indeed, they’re so enthusiastic that sometimes they embrace what’s called neuro myths, misconceptions about how the brain works: for instance, that some children are left brain thinkers and some children at right brain thinkers, or that you can’t pay attention in class unless you’re drinking lots of water.
Some people say that this attempt to translate neuroscience in the education can’t be done, that those two disciplines are too far apart, that we’re taking activity of brain cells and looking at the connections between neurons or perhaps we’re using magnetic resonance imaging to see what parts of the brain are more active, and at the other end we’ve got a classroom full of children who are running around and the teachers trying to get them to sit still and learn something. How can those two things match up with each other? Well, certainly the translation between the low level of neuroscience and actual effective techniques that will work in the classroom is a big translation. There are a lot of steps between them about taking new findings from neuroscience and seeing whether they have educational implication. And then seeing, if there is an apparent implication, say, for how many times you might repeat material in certain ways to make sure that it’s best learned, can we actually put that in a form that it’s going to be useful to teachers? But there are people who say that it can’t be done, that really the proper academics, proper scientists that teachers should be talking to are psychologists and not neuroscientists. I think that there are some risks about that approach.
If you ignore how the brain works and you just stick with psychology and these constructs we’ve come up with, particularly those based on the way a computer works, I think that there are things that just make no sense.
Let me give you some examples. So why is it that I’m trying to learn French vocabulary and I forget it a few months or a few years later but I don’t forget that I’m scared of spiders? Why is it that after a good night sleep I seem to be able to remember what I learned yesterday better, why is that? Why is that the teenagers all of a sudden start making risky decisions and trying to impress their friends, what happens then? Why is it that I seem to be able to learn a new language better when I’m five years of age than when I’m 50 years of age? All of these things have no basis in understanding what a computer does. Another example, why is it that I do lots of learning on my topic, I sit in an exam and I’m really stressed and suddenly my mind goes blank? All of these aspects, things like phobias, things like sleep, things like puberty, things like aging, they have no basis in how a computer works: computers don’t get stressed. So there’s a lot of aspects that appear within children’s behavior, children’s learning that we won’t get any insight into unless we understand how the brain is working.
So this is one of the big puzzles in teaching: when you teach a child or an adult a given skill they just seem to get better on that particular skill. It’s really hard to find activities that you can do that will make people better at everything. In fact, sometimes people seek this like they are Holy Grail, like here is work that says meditation may make you better at everything, learning a musical instrument may improve your intelligence, but these are very much of the exceptions. Mainly when you learn something, you train on something, you only get better on that skill. So the question is why is teaching so specific in its effects? And this is something again that really makes no sense in terms of the idea that the mind works like a desktop computer. The desktop computer is characterized by general mechanisms in terms of a central processing unit, in terms of a hard disk, in terms of the working memory or these days in terms of the cloud: these are mechanisms that process all the kind of content. You would have thought, if you trained your central processing unit, you would get better or all the processing you do. Well, your brain doesn’t work that way. Its content is built into specialized circuits, specific circuits, and that’s why if you improve, if you strengthen the connections between the neurons in one circuit, it doesn’t transfer to the strength of the connections in other circuit. Of course, you’ve got to think how a system like that is going to work with all these specialized components: this one’s for vision, this one’s for hearing, this one’s for decision-making. What you also have there is a control system, one that will activate the right circuits in the right context and inhibit the wrong one.
So the goal of education in that framework is to try and see how we can put the right specialized content into the right systems, but also to train the controlling system, the controller, to make sure that the right parts of the system are activated in the right context. And, of course, we have to bear in mind that the brain is the seat of the emotions as well, and it’s important for children in the classroom to have the emotional stability, security and behavioral regulation to be ready to learn in the classroom.
So the big message of educational neuroscience is that it’s early days, the brain is a very complicated thing, education is very diverse in its goals. But as we understand more about brain mechanisms, there are strong hints that we will be able to transfer that into lessons for learning in the classroom.
So some of the areas where educational neurosciences has made its biggest strides is understanding that the brain circuits and the functionality of skills such as learning to read, such as mathematics, such as doing arithmetic are interesting because children raised in an environment without education just wouldn’t learn those brain structures. So we have this extended special environment of exposing children to letters and written forms, and pronunciation, and reading, and the comparison of reading to pictures and scenes and scenarios that gradually cause our brain structures to build parts of the brain that will be specialized for reading words, so there’s a visual word form area in the brain and it has certain properties, and that only comes about by spending weeks and weeks training children in these culturally determined environments. The same goes for mathematics, for understanding how we represent size, how we represent symbolic number and what happens in children with developmental disorders like dyslexia or dyscalculia that may limit their ability to learn those aspects.
A good example of how you might transfer from an understanding of neuroscience to what it might mean in the classroom is the example of learning about science. What’s interesting about science is in many cases scientific knowledge overwrites intuitive knowledge about how the world works. Take a six-years-old boy: he learns in class that the world is round, it’s a sphere, it’s a curve surface, but for all of his six years he’s been running out around and for maybe two years he’s been playing football on the football pitch that doesn’t look curve, that looks flat. So how does that child go about learning that the Earth is a sphere when his everyday experience says it’s flat? Well, one way you might think is you just overwrite that old knowledge and you put in the new scientific knowledge. Neuroscience studied expert scientists: you put them in the brain scanner and see what’s happening in their brain. It turns out that what they’re doing is actively inhibiting their intuitive knowledge and activating their scientific knowledge. What makes them an expert is they’re better at inhibiting irrelevant intuitive knowledge.
That gives us a hint that one of the skills that might help children learn science is helping them with this suppression or inhibition skill. There’s actually a technique that we’re evaluating here at the Сentre for Educational Neuroscience in London where we’re training eight, nine, ten years old children in the area of science, to learn about these counterintuitive ideas in science and to suppress the irrelevant intuitive knowledge. And we believe that this will help them acquire the broader topic of science and it’s a technique we’re currently evaluating.
So one possibility about very intelligent individuals is that they have very good inhibitory control skills, the ability to really shut off intuitive knowledge, everyday knowledge that is not relevant to the very technical fields where they’re becoming experts.
Neuroscientist Karl Friston on the Markov blanket, Bayesian model evidence, and different global brain theorie...
Researchers find a likely suspect for the neural substrate of subjective happiness
Biologist Catherine Dulac on triggers of social behavior, the genetic breakthrough, and pheromones